Search Weight Loss Topics: |
Urbanization and market integration have strong, nonlinear effects on cardiometabolic health in the Turkana – Science Advances
Abstract
The mismatch between evolved human physiology and Western lifestyles is thought to explain the current epidemic of cardiovascular disease (CVD) in industrialized societies. However, this hypothesis has been difficult to test because few populations concurrently span ancestral and modern lifestyles. To address this gap, we collected interview and biomarker data from individuals of Turkana ancestry who practice subsistence-level, nomadic pastoralism (the ancestral way of life for this group), as well as individuals who no longer practice pastoralism and live in urban areas. We found that Turkana who move to cities exhibit poor cardiometabolic health, partially because of a shift toward Western diets high in refined carbohydrates. We also show that being born in an urban area independently predicts adult health, such that life-long city dwellers will experience the greatest CVD risk. By focusing on a substantial lifestyle gradient, our work thus informs the timing, magnitude, and evolutionary causes of CVD.
It has become increasingly clear that the spread of Western, industrialized lifestyles is contributing to a rapid rise in metabolic and cardiovascular diseases (CVDs) worldwide (15). Since the Industrial Revolution, modern advancements in agriculture, transportation, and manufacture have had a profound impact on human diets and activity patterns, such that calorie-dense food is often easily accessible and adequate nutrition can be achieved with a sedentary lifestyle. This state of affairs, which is typical in Western, industrialized societies but rapidly spreading across developing countries, stands in stark contrast to the ecological conditions experienced over the vast majority of our evolutionary history. Consequently, the mismatch between human physiologywhich evolved to cope with a mixed plant- and meat-based diet, activity-intensive foraging, and periods of resource scarcityand Western, industrialized lifestyles has been hypothesized to explain the current epidemic of cardiometabolic disease (14).
Attempts to test the evolutionary mismatch hypothesis thus far have largely focused on comparing cardiometabolic health outcomes between industrialized nations and small-scale, subsistence-level groups (e.g., hunter-gatherers, horticulturalists, and pastoralists). Arguably, the diets and activity patterns of these subsistence-level groups are relatively in line with their recent evolutionary history, and these populations can thus be thought of as matched to their evolutionary past (1, 5). In support of the evolutionary mismatch hypothesis, essentially, all subsistence-level populations studied to date show minimal type 2 diabetes, hypertension, obesity, and heart disease relative to the United States and Europe (513). Two other classes of studies provide further support: (i) Indigenous populations that have recently transitioned to market-based economies show higher rates of obesity and metabolic syndrome compared to subsistence-level groups [e.g., (14, 15)] and (ii) comparisons between rural and urban areas in developing countries have found higher rates of hypertension, type 2 diabetes, and obesity in the urban, industrialized setting (1620).
Despite the groundwork that has been laid so far in understanding how Western lifestyles influence health, most prior studies leave two major gaps. First, the participants genetic backgrounds are either heterogeneous (in the case of urban versus rural comparisons within a country) or confounded with lifestyle (in the case of subsistence-level versus U.S. or Europe comparisons). This makes it difficult to disentangle genetic versus environmental contributions to health. A more robust study design would be to compare health between individuals living their ancestral, traditional way of life versus individuals from the same genetic background living a modern, industrialized lifestyle. This type of natural experiment is difficult to come by [but see (13)], and large-scale work that has assessed acculturation effects on cardiometabolic health within a single group has therefore been limited to more modest lifestyle gradients [e.g., work with the Tsimane (15, 17), Shuar (10), or Yakut (18, 21)]. A second major gap is that research to date has focused on industrialization and acculturation effects at particular life stages, mainly in adulthood, despite strong evidence that early-life conditions influence adult health and that life-course perspectives are likely important (19, 20). Of particular relevance is the hypothesis that individuals use cues during development to predict what the adult environment will be like and develop phenotypes well suited for those conditions. Under such a predictive adaptive response (PAR) framework, industrial transitions are thought to be especially detrimental because individuals may be born in resource-poor environments but exposed to resource-rich environments as adults; individuals are thus phenotypically prepared for scarcity but encounter plenty instead, leading to a within-lifetime environmental mismatch and subsequent cardiometabolic disease (2224). Despite the popularity and potential significance of this idea, little work has robustly and empirically tested it against other evolutionary explanations for why early-life resource scarcity is commonly associated with poor adult cardiometabolic health (20, 2528). In particular, the developmental constraints (DC) hypothesis alternatively predicts that early-life nutritional challenges will be unavoidably costly and associated with poor health outcomes no matter the adult environment (20, 25, 28).
To address these gaps, we collected interviews and cardiometabolic health biomarker data from the Turkana, a subsistence-level, pastoralist population from a remote desert in northwest Kenya (Fig. 1) (29, 30). The Turkana and their ancestors have practiced nomadic pastoralism in arid regions of East Africa for thousands of years (30), and present-day, traditional Turkana still rely on livestock for subsistence: 62% of calories are derived from fresh or fermented milk, and another 12% of calories come from animal meat, fat, or blood (29) [specifically, the Turkana herd dromedary camels, zebu cattle, fat-tailed sheep, goats, and donkeys (29)]. The remaining calories are derived from wild foods or products obtained through occasional trade (e.g., cereals, tea, and oil) (29). However, as infrastructure in Kenya has improved in the past few decades, small-scale markets have expanded into northwest Kenya, leading some Turkana to no longer practice nomadic pastoralism and to rely more heavily on the market economy; specifically, these individuals make and sell charcoal or woven baskets or keep animals in a fixed location for trade rather than subsistence. In addition to the emergence of this nonpastoralist (but still relatively subsistence-level) subgroup, some individuals have left the Turkana homelands entirely and now live in highly urbanized parts of central Kenya (Fig. 1). The Turkana situation thus presents a unique opportunity, in that individuals of the same genetic background can be found across a substantial lifestyle gradient ranging from relatively matched to extremely mismatched with their recent evolutionary history. Further, because many Turkana are currently migrating between rural and urban areas within their lifetime, we were able to empirically test the PAR hypothesis by asking whether individuals who experienced rural conditions in early life but urban conditions in adulthood exhibited worse cardiometabolic health than individuals whose early and adult environments were similar. We tested this idea against the DC hypothesis, which predicts that early-life challenges incur simple long-term costs that are not contingent on the adult environment (20, 25, 28).
(A) Sampling locations throughout northern and central Kenya are marked with red dots; the county borders are marked with dashed lines. In both Laikipia and Turkana counties, the largest city (which is generally central within each county) is marked with a black dot. (B) Schematic describing the three lifestyle groups that were sampled as part of this study. (C) The proportion of people from each lifestyle group who reported that they consumed a particular item regularly, defined as one to two times per week, more than two times per week, or every day. People who reported that they consumed a particular item rarely or never were categorized as not consuming the item regularly. Animal products are a staple of the traditional pastoralist diet (85), while carbohydrates and added nutrients, which can only be obtained through trade, are indicative of market integration.
Capitalizing on this natural experiment, we sampled 1226 adult Turkana in 44 locations from the following groups: (i) individuals practicing subsistence-level pastoralism in the Turkana homelands, (ii) individuals that do not practice pastoralism but live in the same remote, rural area, and (iii) individuals living in urban centers (Fig. 1). We found that cardiometabolic profiles across 10 biomarkers were favorable in pastoralist Turkana, and rates of obesity and metabolic syndrome were low, similar to other subsistence-level populations (612). Comparisons within the Turkana revealed a nonlinear relationship between the extent of industrialization or evolutionary mismatch and cardiometabolic health: No significant biomarker differences were found between pastoralists and nonpastoralists from rural areas. However, we found strong, sometimes sex-specific, differences in health between these two groups and nonpastoralists living in urban areas, although metabolic dysfunction among urban Turkana did not reach the levels observed in the United States. Using formal mediation analyses (31, 32), we show that consumption of processed, calorically dense foods (primarily carbohydrates and cooking fats) and indices of market integration may explain health shifts in urban Turkana. Last, we show that a proxy of urbanization (population density) experienced around the time of birth is associated with worse adult cardiometabolic health, independent of adult lifestyle. In other words, the health costs of living an industrialized lifestyle in early life and adulthood are additive, such that within-lifetime environmental mismatches do not appear to exacerbate health issues as has been previously suggested (22, 24).
To characterize the health of the Turkana people, we collected extensive interview and biomarker data from adult Turkana sampled throughout Kenya (Table 1 and Fig. 1). We measured body mass index (BMI), waist circumference, total cholesterol, triglycerides, high- and low-density lipoproteins (HDLs and LDLs), body fat percentage, systolic and diastolic blood pressure, and blood glucose levels (Table 2). We also created a composite measure of health, defined as the proportion of measured biomarkers that exceed cutoffs set by the U.S. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) or the American Heart Association as being indicative of disease (see Supplementary Materials and Methods).
NHANES, National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey; MI, market integration. M, Male; F, Female.
BP, blood pressure.
As has been observed in other subsistence-level populations (5), we found extremely low levels of cardiometabolic disease among traditional, pastoralist Turkana: No individuals met the criteria for obesity (BMI > 30) or metabolic syndrome (33), and only 6.4% of individuals had hypertension [blood pressure > 135/85 (33)]. Further, across eight cardiometabolic biomarkers that have been measured consistently in other subsistence-level populations (612), the means observed in the Turkana were generally within the range of what others have reported (table S1, A and B). Mean body fat percentage (mean SD for females = 20.45 4.57%) and BMI (19.99 2.14 kg/m2) were on the lower extremes but were similar to other pastoralists (mean BMI in the Fulani and the Maasai = 20.2 and 20.7 kg/m2, respectively) and to a small study of the Turkana conducted in the 1980s [mean BMI = 17.7 kg/m2 (34)]. Notably, the only biomarkers that were strongly differentiated in traditional, pastoralist Turkana were HDL (72.69 14.72 mg/dl) and LDL cholesterol levels (60.89 20.22 mg/dl), both of which were even more favorable than what has been observed in other subsistence-level groups, including the Fulani and the Maasai (range of reported means for HDL = 34.45 to 49.11 mg/dl and LDL = 72.70 to 92.81 mg/dl). It remains to be seen why the Turkana HDL/LDL profiles appear as strong and consistent outliers relative to other subsistence-level groups, but one possibility is that there has been selection on Turkana lipid traits as a result of their unique diet, ecology, and lifestyle. This possibility could be explored in future evolutionary genetic and metabolic studies.
Next, we sought to understand the shape of the relationship between industrialization and cardiometabolic health within the Turkana, by comparing biomarker values across the three lifestyle categories. Using linear models controlling for age and sex, we found that Turkana practicing traditional pastoralism did not differ in any of the 10 measured biomarkers relative to nonpastoralist Turkana living in similarly rural areas (all P values > 0.05; Fig. 2 and table S2A), despite there being major dietary difference between these groups (Fig. 1). Pastoralist and rural nonpastoralist Turkana did significantly differ in our composite measure, with nonpastoralist Turkana exhibiting more biomarker values above clinical cutoffs [average proportion of biomarkers above cutoffs = 4.02 and 6.82% for pastoralists and nonpastoralists, respectively; P value = 1.39 103; false discovery rate (FDR) < 5%; Fig. 2].
(A) Effect sizes for contrasts between pastoralist, rural nonpastoralist, and urban nonpastoralist Turkana (from linear models controlling for age and sex; table S2A). Effect sizes are standardized, such that the x axis represents the difference in terms of SDs between groups. BP, blood pressure. (B) Standardized effect sizes for contrasts between rural Turkana (pastoralist and rural nonpastoralist grouped together), urban nonpastoralist Turkana, and the U.S. (from linear models controlling for age and sex; table S2B). In (A) and (B), lighter colored bars represent effect sizes that were not significant [false discovery rate (FDR) > 5%], and analyses of body fat and blood glucose focus on females only (see Supplementary Materials and Methods). Symbols correspond to FDR significance thresholds as follows: *FDR < 0.1%, FDR < 1%, and +FDR < 5%. (C) Predicted values for a typical rural Turkana (pastoralist and rural nonpastoralist grouped together), urban Turkana, and U.S. individual are shown for a subset of significant biomarkers. Estimates and error bars were obtained using coefficients and their SEs from fitted models, for a female of average age (see Supplementary Materials and Methods).
Notably, biomarker values for both pastoralist and nonpastoralist, rural Turkana were consistently more favorable than among Turkana living in urban areas in central Kenya. People living in urban areas exhibited composite measures indicative of worse cumulative cardiometabolic health (average proportion of biomarkers above cutoffs = 13.42%), higher BMIs and body fat percentages, larger waist circumferences, higher blood pressure, and higher levels of total cholesterol, triglycerides, and blood glucose (all FDR < 5%; Fig. 2 and table S2A). The only tested variables that did not exhibit differences between urban and rural Turkana (both pastoralists and nonpastoralists) were the HDL and LDL cholesterol levels, which were favorable in all Turkana regardless of lifestyle (tables S1A and S2A). Using standardized effect sizes, we found that the biomarkers that differed most between the two rural groups and urban residents were blood glucose, triglycerides, and BMI (Fig. 2). For example, the average urban Turkana resident has a 9.69 and 8.43% higher BMI relative to pastoralist and nonpastoralist rural Turkana, respectively.
For all 11 measures, we explored the possibility of age by lifestyle category and sex by lifestyle category interactions. We found no evidence that age modifies the response to lifestyle change (FDR > 5% for all biomarkers; likelihood ratio test comparing models with versus without the interaction term). However, we did find that inclusion of a sex by lifestyle category term improved the model fit for blood glucose levels (P value from a likelihood ratio test = 1.838 104) and body fat percentage (P value = 4.234 103; table S2A). In both cases, women experienced worse health with increasing market integration and industrialization, while men did not (fig. S1). The nature of this interaction is consistent with several previous studies (10, 21, 35, 36); however, the specific reasons behind the heightened sensitivity of women to acculturation (in our study and elsewhere) remain unknown. Previous work has speculated that these sex-specific effects are driven by social and behavioral factors that affect diet and activity patterns (e.g., rate of acquisition of wage labor jobs) and that change more markedly for women versus men during industrial transitions (10). It is likely that this general explanation applies to the Turkana as well, although follow-up work is needed to understand the specifics.
We next asked whether the biomarker levels observed among urban Turkana approached those observed in a fully Western, industrialized society (specifically, the United States). We note that a caveat of these analyses is that they must include different genetic backgrounds since Turkana individuals are rarely found in fully industrialized countries.
To compare metabolic health between the U.S., rural Turkana (grouping pastoralists and nonpastoralists since these groups were minimally differentiated in previous analyses), and urban Turkana, we downloaded data from the CDCs National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) conducted in 2006 (37), focusing on adults (ages 18-65) to recapitulate the age distribution of our Turkana dataset (see Table 1 for sample sizes). Comparisons between NHANES and our Turkana dataset revealed that, while urban Turkana exhibit biomarker values indicative of poorer health than rural Turkana, urban Turkana have more favorable metabolic profiles than the U.S. (Fig. 2, figs. S2 and S3, and table S2B). This pattern held for all measures except (i) blood glucose levels, where no differences were observed (P value for U.S. versus rural Turkana = 0.166, U.S. versus urban Turkana = 0.074); (ii) triglycerides, where urban Turkana could not be distinguished from the U.S. (P = 0.627); (iii) systolic blood pressure, where urban Turkana exhibited higher values than the U.S. (4.55% higher; P = 3.02 106; FDR < 5%); and (iv) diastolic blood pressure, where mean values for both urban and rural Turkana were unexpectedly higher than the U.S. (rural Turkana, 8.77% higher than U.S., P = 4.43 1065; urban Turkana, 11.69% higher than U.S., P = 3.58 1031, FDR < 5% for both comparisons; fig. S4). These differences in diastolic blood pressure remained after removing all U.S. individuals taking cardiometabolic medications (rural Turkana, 6.91% higher than U.S., P = 3.27 1067; urban Turkana, 10.37% higher than U.S., P = 2.21 1035). However, two pieces of evidence suggest that the higher diastolic blood pressure values observed in the Turkana are not pathological: (i) Values for rural Turkana (77.43 15.22 mmHg) are similar to estimates from other subsistence-level populations without cardiometabolic disease (range of published means = 70.9 to 79.9 mmHg; table S1A) and (ii) few rural Turkana meet the criteria for hypertension relative to the U.S. (fig. S3). Future work is needed to understand the environmental and/or genetic sources of the observed differences in blood pressure between Turkana and U.S. individuals.
For measures that exhibited differences between urban Turkana and the U.S. in the expected directions, these effect sizes were consistently much larger in magnitude than the differences that we observed between rural and urban Turkana (Fig. 2). For example, while the average urban Turkana experiences a 9% higher BMI than their rural counterparts, the average U.S. individual has a BMI that is 44 and 32% higher than rural and urban Turkana, respectively. Similarly, while the average proportion of biomarkers above clinical cutoffs is 6.22% in rural Turkana and 13.42% in urban Turkana, this number rises to 38.84% in the U.S.
We next sought to identify the specific dietary, lifestyle, or environmental inputs that drive differential health outcomes between urban and rural Turkana. To do so, we turned to interview data collected for each individual (see Supplementary Materials and Methods), which revealed substantial variation in diet, market access, and urbanicity [a term that we use to mean living in an urbanized area and engaging in an urban lifestyle, following (38); Figs. 1 and 3]. We paired these interview data with mediation analyses (31, 32) to formally test whether the effect of a predictor (X) on an outcome (Y) was direct or, instead, indirectly explained by a third variable (M) such that XMY (Fig. 3). Using this statistical framework, we tested whether the following factors could explain the decline in metabolic health observed in urban Turkana: increased consumption of market-derived, calorically dense foods (e.g., carbohydrates such as soda, bread, rice, as well as fats such as cooking oil), reduced consumption of traditional animal products, poorer health habits, ownership of more market-derived goods and modern amenities (e.g., cell phone, finished floor, and electricity), occupation that is more market integrated (e.g., formal employment), and residence in a more populated or developed area (measured via population density, distance to a major city, and female education levels) (see Supplementary Materials and Methods). In particular, we predicted that lifestyle effects on health would be mediated by a shift toward a diet that incorporates more carbohydrates and fewer animal products in urban Turkana. These analyses focused on biomarkers for which our sample sizes were the largest since dietary data were not available for all individuals (see table S3 for sample sizes).
(A) Key measures of urbanicity and market integration used in mediation analyses, with means and distributions shown for urban and rural Turkana. (B) Schematic of mediation analyses. Specifically, mediation analyses test the hypothesis that lifestyle effects on health are explained by an intermediate variable, such as consumption of particular food items (red arrows); alternatively, lifestyle effects on health may be direct (black arrow) or mediated by a variable that we did not measure. (C) Summary of mediation analysis results, where colored squares indicate a variable that was found to significantly explain urban-rural health differences in a given biomarker. Significant mediators are colored on the basis of how much the lifestyle effect (urban/rural) decreased when a given mediator was included in the model. MI, market integration. Full results and sample sizes for mediation analyses are presented in table S3.
In support of our predictions, urban-rural differences in waist circumference, BMI, and our composite measure of health were mediated by greater consumption of processed, calorically dense foods (including carbohydrates and fats) and lower consumption of traditional animal products (milk and blood) in urban Turkana (Fig. 3 and table S3). Notably, the total number of different carbohydrate items that an individual consumed was a strong and consistent predictor across these three biomarkers, suggesting that individual dietary components may matter less than overall exposure to refined carbohydrates. However, it is important to note that refined carbohydrates are commonly processed with oil or other additives, and it is therefore likely a combined effect of exposure to both carbohydrates and fats that drives the negative health effects that we observe.
Contrary to our predictions, we did not find that dietary differences mediated urban-rural differences in systolic blood pressure, diastolic blood pressure, or body fat percentage. Instead, these measures were explained by variables that captured how industrialized and market-integrated a given individuals lifestyle was, which was also important for waist circumference, BMI, and our composite measure of health in addition to dietary effects. For example, fine-scale measures of population density and degree of market reliance of occupation both significantly mediated five of six tested biomarkers (Fig. 3 and table S3). Further, these indices of urbanicity and market integration tended to be stronger mediators than dietary variables (Fig. 3).
To understand the degree to which the mediators that we identified explain the relationship between lifestyle and a given biomarker, we compared the magnitude of the lifestyle effect in our original models (controlling for age and sex, without any mediators) to the effect estimated in the presence of all significant mediators. If the mediators fully explain the relationship between lifestyle and a given biomarker, then we would expect the estimate of the lifestyle effect to be zero in the second model. These analyses revealed that the mediators that we identified explain most of the relationship between lifestyle and waist circumference (effect size decrease = 90.7%), BMI (79.9%), systolic blood pressure (74.9%), and composite health (64.1%) but explain only a small portion of lifestyle effects on diastolic blood pressure (10.0%) and body fat (23.5%; table S3).
Last, we were interested in understanding whether early-life conditions had long-term effects on health, above and beyond the effects of adult lifestyle that we had already identified. We were motivated to ask this question because work in humans and nonhuman animals has demonstrated strong associations between diet and ecology during the first years of life and fitness-related traits measured many years later (20, 25, 39, 40). Two major hypotheses have been proposed to explain why this embedding of early-life conditions into long-term health occurs. First, the PAR hypothesis posits that organisms adjust their phenotype during development in anticipation of predicted adult conditions. Individuals that encounter adult environments that match their early conditions are predicted to gain a selective advantage, whereas animals that encounter mismatched adult environments should suffer a fitness cost (19, 22, 25, 41, 42). In contrast, the DC or silver spoon hypothesis predicts a simple relationship between early environmental quality and adult fitness: Individuals born in high-quality environments experience a fitness advantage regardless of the adult environment (25, 28, 43). Under DC, poor-quality early-life conditions cannot be ameliorated by matching adult and early-life environments; instead, the effects of environmental adversity accumulate across the life course.
We found no evidence that individuals who experienced matched early-life and adult environments had better metabolic health in adulthood than individuals who experienced mismatched early-life and adult conditions (P > 0.05 for all biomarkers). In particular, we tested for interaction effects between the population density of each individuals birth location (estimated for their year of birth) and a binary factor indicating whether the adult environment was urban or rural (table S4A; see table S4B for parallel analyses using population density to define the adult environment as a continuous measure). This analysis was possible given the within-lifetime migrations of many Turkana between urban and rural areas: Only 19.52 and 33.01% of urban and rural Turkana, respectively, were sampled within 10 km of their birthplace, and the correlation between birth and sampling location population densities was low (R2 = 0.115; P < 1016; fig. S5).
While we observed no evidence for interaction effects supporting PAR, we did find strong main effects of early-life population density on adult waist circumference (b = 0.272, P = 7.33 108), BMI (b = 0.266, P = 1.35 107), body fat (b = 0.306, P = 1.57 105), diastolic blood pressure (b = 0.124, P = 2.01 102), and our composite measure of health (b = 0.296, P = 3.40 104; all FDR < 5%), in support of DC. For all biomarkers, being born in a densely populated location was associated with poorer adult metabolic health (Fig. 4 and table S4A). Furthermore, the early-life environment effect was on the same order of magnitude as the effect of living in an urban versus rural location in adulthood (see table S4A and Supplementary Materials and Methods). For example, BMIs are 5.69% higher in urban versus rural areas, while individuals born in areas from the 25th versus 75th percentile of the early-life population density distribution exhibit BMIs that differ by 3.34%. Similarly, the effect of the adult environment (urban compared to rural) on female body fat percentages is 11.14%, while the effect of early-life population density (25th compared to 75th percentile) is 20.7% (table S4A).
The relationship between the population density of each individuals birth location and (A) BMI, (B) our composite measure of health, (C) waist circumference, and (D) diastolic blood pressure are shown for individuals sampled in rural and urban locations, respectively. Notably, while the intercept for a linear fit between early-life population density and each biomarker differs between rural and urban sampling locations (indicating mean differences in biomarker values as a function of adult lifestyle), the slope of the line does not. In other words, we find no evidence that the relationship between early-life conditions and adult health is contingent on the adult environmnt (as predicted by PAR). Instead, being born in an urban location predicts poorer metabolic health regardless of the adult environment.
By sampling a relatively endogamous population across a substantial lifestyle gradient, we show that (i) traditional, pastoralist Turkana exhibit low levels of cardiometabolic disease and (ii) increasing industrialization, in both early life and adulthood, has detrimental, additive effects on metabolic health (in opposition of popular PAR models that have rarely been tested empirically in humans) (20, 22, 24). Our findings offer strong support for the evolutionary mismatch hypothesis, more so than existing studies that cannot disentangle lifestyle and genetic background effects (612, 44, 45, 46) or that assess lifestyle effects across much more modest gradients (10, 17, 21, 47, 48). Our work also provides some of the first multidimensional, large-scale data on acculturation and industrialization effects on cardiometabolic health in pastoralists [see also (34, 49, 50)], which have received less attention than other subsistence modes [e.g., horticulturalists such as the Shuar and Tsimane (10, 15, 51, 52)].
Our observation that pastoralist Turkana do not suffer from cardiometabolic diseases echoes long-standing findings from other subsistence-level groups (612). However, it also provides empirical support for a more recent and controversial hypothesis: that many types of mixed plant- and meat-based diets are compatible with cardiometabolic health (1, 53, 54) and that mismatches between the distant human hunter-gatherer past and the subsistence-level practices of horticulturalists or pastoralists do not lead to disease (55). In other words, contemporary hunter-gatherers are most aligned with human subsistence practices that evolved ~300 thousand years ago (56), but they do not exhibit better cardiometabolic health relative to horticulturalists or pastoralists, whose subsistence practices evolved ~12 thousand years ago (tables S1A to S3) (57). Instead, we find evidence consistent with the idea that extreme mismatches between the recent evolutionary history of a population and lifestyle are needed to produce the chronic diseases now prevalent worldwide; in the Turkana, this situation appears to manifest in urban, industrialized areas but not in rural areas with changing livelihoods but limited access to the market economy.
Because our study assessed health in individuals who experience no, limited, or substantial access to the market economy, we were able to determine that industrialization has nonlinear effects on health in the Turkana. In particular, we find no differences between pastoralists and nonpastoralists in rural areas for 10 of 11 variables (Fig. 2), despite nonpastoralists consuming processed carbohydrates that are atypical of traditional practices (Fig. 1 and table S1A). Nevertheless, rural nonpastoralists still live in remote areas, engage in activity-intensive subsistence activities, and rely far less heavily on markets than urban Turkana. Given the mosaic of lifestyle factors that can change with modernization, often in concert, our results suggest that this type of lifestyle has not crossed the threshold necessary to produce cardiometabolic health issues. This threshold model may help explain heterogeneity in previous studies, where small degrees of evolutionary mismatch and market integration have produced inconsistent changes in cardiometabolic health biomarkers (10, 58, 59).
While our dataset does not capture every variable that mediates urban-rural health differences in the Turkana, we were able to account for a substantial portion (>60%) of lifestyle effects on waist circumference, BMI, systolic blood pressure, and composite health. In line with our expectations, increases in these biomarkers in urban areas were mediated by greater reliance on processed, calorically dense foods (i.e., refined carbohydrates and cooking fats) and reduced consumption of animal products (Figs. 1 and 3). However, our mediation analyses also show that broader measures of lifestyle modernization (e.g., population density, distance to a major city, and female education levels) have stronger explanatory power than diet alone. It is likely that these indices serve as proxies for unmeasured, more proximate mediators, such as psychosocial stress, nutrient balance, total caloric intake, or total energy expenditure, all of which vary by industrialization and can affect health (1, 12, 6063). The fact that the number of meals eaten per day (which is typically one in rural areas and two to three in urban areas) was a strong mediator for three of six variables points to total caloric intake, while the importance of occupation suggests activity levels are also probably key. More generally and as expected, our mediation analyses suggest that the link between lifestyle change and health is complex, multifactorial (e.g., driven by a suite of dietary and other factors), and potentially quite different for different biomarkers. Work with the Turkana is under way to address some of the unmeasured sources of variance that we hypothesize to be especially critical, namely, total caloric intake and total energy expenditure.
In addition to the pervasive influence of adult lifestyle on metabolic physiology that we observe in the Turkana, our analyses also revealed appreciable effects of early-life environments. In particular, controlling for the adult environment (urban or rural), birth location population density was a significant predictor of BMI, waist circumference, diastolic blood pressure, our composite measure of health, and body fat. Further, the impact of early-life and adult conditions appears to be on the same order of magnitude, although they do, in some cases, vary up to twofold. In particular, we observed 2 to 6% differences in BMI, waist circumference, and diastolic blood pressure, as a function of each life stage, while body fat and our composite measure show changes in the 11 to 20% range (note, however, that the measures that we used to quantify early-life and adult conditions are not the same, making direct effect size comparisons difficult; table S4A).
We did not find evidence that individuals who grew up in rural versus urban conditions were more prepared for these environments later in life, as predicted by PAR. These findings agree with work in preindustrial human populations and long-lived mammals, which have found weak or no support for PARs (26, 27, 6466). Together, these findings suggest that because early-life ecological conditions are often a poor predictor of adult environments for long-lived organisms, a strategy matching individual physiology to an unpredictable adult environment is unlikely to evolve (6769). Instead, our work joins others in concluding that challenging early-life environments simply incur long-term health costs (26, 27, 6466). While previous work in subsistence-level groups has clearly shown an effect of early-life environments (including acculturation) on health outcomes (52, 59), it has not explicitly tested whether PAR versus DC models explain these associations. Our attempt to do so here in the context of urbanicity exposure suggests that rapid industrial transitions are unlikely to create health problems because of within-lifetime environmental mismatches (26, 27, 70). Instead, our findings suggest that greater cumulative exposure to urban, industrialized environments across the life course will create the largest burdens of cardiometabolic disease.
Our study has several limitations. First, with the exception of our biomarker measurements, most of our data are self-reported. It is possible that recall may be imperfect, answers may be exaggerated to appear impressive (e.g., in interviews about the ownership of market goods), or participants may not wish to reveal private details (e.g., in interviews about health habits or covariates). On the basis of our conversations with study participants, we expect intentionally provided misinformation to be rare, but there are two areas where self-reporting contributes to specific limitations worthy of discussion. First, because our age data are self-reported, this key covariate is likely noisy and more so for rural than for urban participants (who are more likely to know their exact birth date). We do not have a reason to believe that this issue affects our estimate of the lifestyle-cardiometabolic health relationship, but it does likely complicate our ability to identify age by lifestyle interactions, which is a critical area for future study. Second, because our diet data are self-reported, we are currently unable to tease apart the precise nutritional components that drive health variation in the Turkana. Our mediation analyses reveal that several market-derived foods are key contributors, suggesting that broad exposure to processed, high-energy foods (including both carbohydrates and fats) is important for cardiometabolic health. Future work that estimates the total caloric intake and the intake of fat, protein, carbohydrates, and micronutrients [as in (71)] in the Turkana are planned.
A second limitation is that we do not know how our biomarker values are related to outcomes such as heart disease or mortality in the Turkana. We are relying on work in Western cohorts that has related lipid profiles, blood glucose, blood pressure, and measures of adiposity to these outcomes (72), but it is possible that those relationships are different in the Turkana [e.g., work in the United States has already demonstrated how the shape of the BMI-mortality curve may differ by race/ethnicity (73)]. Further, certain biomarkers may not linearly track disease and mortality risk: Notably, in Western cohorts, the effect of BMI on all-cause mortality risk is J shaped, such that underweight individuals experience some increase in risk, normal BMI individuals experience the lowest risk, and overweight and obese individuals experience the greatest risk (74). It is therefore difficult to draw conclusions about the relationship between any one biomarker, lifestyle change, and long-term outcomes in the present study. However, two pieces of evidence suggest that the changing biomarker profiles that we observe in urban Turkana are meaningful. First, work in Western countries has consistently shown that when individuals simultaneously cross clinical thresholds for several biomarkers, as we observe in urban Turkana, risk of cardiovascular events and all-cause mortality increases (72). Second, Kenya has seen a marked rise in CVD in recent decades, with 13% of hospital deaths in 2014 attributed to CVD. CVD risk is much higher in urban relative to rural areas across the country, with hypertension and type 2 diabetes estimated to be at least fourfold more prevalent in urban settings (75).
Last, a third limitation of our study is that we lack data on several key factors known to modify or mediate the relationship between lifestyle change and cardiometabolic health, such as total energy expenditure (1), total caloric intake and nutritional composition of the diet (71), and parasite load (76). Our ongoing research with the Turkana is in the process of gathering data on these sources of variance.
The hypothesis that mismatches between evolved human physiology and Western lifestyles cause disease has become a central tenet of evolutionary medicine, with potentially profound implications for how we study, manage, and treat a long list of conditions thought to arise from evolutionary mismatch (77). However, this hypothesis has been difficult to robustly test in practice because of inadequate population comparisons and the multiple types of mismatch to be considered. Leveraging the lifestyle change currently occurring in the Turkana population, we show that cardiometabolic health is worse in urban relative to rural areas but that small deviations from traditional, ancestral practices in rural areas do not produce health effects. To build upon our results, we advocate for more within-population comparisons spanning large lifestyle gradients, combined with longitudinal sampling designs [e.g., (72)]. Longitudinal study of other populations undergoing industrial transitions would also be invaluable for assessing the generality of the early-life effects on adult cardiometabolic health that we observe here and for identifying the specific early-life ecological, social, or behavioral factors that drive long-term variation in health.
Data were collected between April 2018 and March 2019 in Turkana and Laikipia counties in Kenya. During this time, researchers visited locations where individuals of Turkana ancestry were known to reside (Fig. 1). At each sampling location, healthy adults (>18 years old) of self-reported Turkana ancestry were invited to participate in the study, which involved a structured interview and measurement of 10 cardiometabolic biomarkers. Participation rates of eligible adults were high (>75%). GPS coordinates were recorded on a handheld Garmin GPSMAP 64 device at each sampling location. Additional details on the sampling procedures can be found in Supplementary Materials and Methods.
This study was approved by Princeton Universitys Institutional Review Board for Human Subjects Research (Institutional Review Board no. 10237) and Maseno Universitys Ethics Review Committee (MSU/DRPI/MUERC/00519/18). We also received county-level approval from both Laikipia and Turkana counties for research activities and research permits from Kenyas National Commission for Science, Technology, and Innovation (NACOSTI/P/18/46195/24671). Written, informed consent was obtained from all participants after the study goals, sampling procedures, and potential risks were discussed with community elders and explained to participants in their native language (by both a local official, usually the village chief, and by researchers or field assistants).
Individuals were excluded from analyses if they met any of the following criteria: (i) pregnancy, (ii) extreme outlier values for a given biomarker, (iii) missing data on primary subsistence activity, (iv) missing interview data, and (v) missing gender or age. For the early-life effects analyses, we also excluded individuals that did not report a birth location or for whom GPS coordinates for the reported birth location could not be identified on a map. Those missing birth locations had similar health profiles as individuals for whom a birth location could be assigned (all FDR > 5% for linear models testing for an effect of birth location missingness on each biomarker, controlling for age, sex, and lifestyle; table S4C). Thus, although the sample sizes for our early-life effects analyses are smaller than for analyses focused on current environmental/lifestyle effects (table S4A), this sample size reduction is not systematically biased in a way that is likely to affect the results.
Before statistical analyses, all biomarkers (except the composite measure of health) were mean centered and scaled by their SD, using the scale function in R (78). Consequently, all reported effect sizes are standardized and represent the effect of a given variable on the outcome in terms of increases in SDs.
Testing for lifestyle effects on measured biomarkers. For each of the 10 measured biomarkers, we used the following linear model to test for effects of lifestyle controlling for covariatesyi=0+lil+aia+sis+ei(1)where yi is the normalized (mean centered and scaled by the SD) biomarker value for individual i, li is lifestyle (pastoralist; nonpastoralist, rural; or nonpastoralist, urban), ai is age (in years), si is sex (male or female), and ei represents residual error. To determine whether a given biomarker exhibited a lifestyle by sex interaction, we used a likelihood ratio test to compare the fit of model 1 with the following modelyi=0+lil+aia+sis+(lisi)ls+ei(2)
In model 2, l s represents a lifestyle by sex interaction effect. If the P value for the likelihood ratio test comparing models 1 and 2 was less than 0.05, then we concluded that a lifestyle by sex interaction existed, and we tested for the effects of lifestyle within each gender separately (controlling for age). These analyses revealed lifestyle associations with body fat and blood glucose in females but not males (fig. S1). All additional analyses for these biomarkers therefore analyzed data from females alone.
Analyses of blood glucose levels included a covariate noting whether the individual had fasted overnight before the time of blood collection (which was always in the morning). For analyses of the composite measure of health, we used the same approach and the same main and interaction effects described for models 1 and 2 paired with generalized linear models with a binomial link function to accommodate count data. Specifically, the composite measure of health was modeled as the number of biomarkers that exceeded clinical cutoffs/the number of biomarkers measured for a given individual. Only individuals with three or more measured biomarkers were included in this analysis.
For all 11 measures (10 biomarkers and 1 composite measure), we extracted the P values associated with the lifestyle effect (l) from our models and corrected for multiple hypothesis testing using a Benjamini-Hochberg FDR (79). We considered a given lifestyle contrast to be significant if the FDR-corrected P value was less than 0.05 (equivalent to a 5% FDR threshold). The results of all final models are presented in table S2A.
Identifying factors that mediate lifestyle effects on measured biomarkers. For each biomarker that was significantly associated with lifestyle (table S2A), we were interested in identifying specific variables that mediated urban-rural differences in health. However, we did not perform mediation analyses for lipid traits and for blood glucose, as sample sizes for these biomarkers were smaller to begin with, and, after overlapping with our dietary data, we could only include 50 to 60 urban individuals. Therefore, we focused mediation analyses on waist circumference, BMI, diastolic and systolic blood pressure, body fat, and our composite measure of health (sample sizes for mediation analyses are presented in table S3).
To implement mediation analyses, we used an approach similar to (80, 81) to estimate the indirect effect of lifestyle on a given biomarker through the following potential mediating variables: alcohol and tobacco use (yes/no); consumption of meat, milk, blood, cooking oil, sugar, salt, bread, rice, ugali, potatoes, soda, fried foods, and sweets (frequency of use measured on a 0 to 4 scale); total number of unique carbohydrate items consumed; number of meals eaten per day; distance to the nearest city (in kilometers); log10 population density; main subsistence activity; proportion of mothers in the sampling location with no formal education; and a tally of the number of market-derived amenities an individual had (see Supplementary Materials and Methods). Occupation was coded to reflect integration in the market economy as follows: 0 = animal keeping, farming, fishing, hunting, and gathering; 1 = charcoal burning and mat making; 2 = casual worker, petty trade, and self-employment; and 3 = formal employment. To estimate female education levels in a given area, we calculated the fraction of women sampled in a given area with >0 children who reported having received no education. Population density, distance to a city, the proportion of mothers with no formal education, and the number of owned market goods are all measures that have been used in the literature to describe how urban a given individual/location is (82, 83). Population density estimates were derived from NASAs Socioeconomic Data and Applications Center (https://doi.org/10.7927/H49C6VHW). Specifically, we used the Gridded Population of the World database (version 4.11) to estimate the number of persons per square kilometer for each sampling location based on our GPS coordinates (see the Supplementary Materials).
For all mediation analyses, we used two categories to describe lifestyle, urban and rural, given minimal health differences between pastoralist and nonpastoralist Turkana living in rural areas. For biomarkers with no sex by lifestyle interaction, we estimated the strength of the indirect effect of each mediator as the difference between the effect of lifestyle (urban versus rural) in two linear models: the unadjusted model that did not account for the mediator (equivalent to model 1 in the Testing for lifestyle effects on measured biomarkers section) and the effect of lifestyle in an adjusted model that also incorporated the mediator. If a given variable is a strong mediator, then the effect of lifestyle will decrease when this variable is included in the model and absorbs variance otherwise attributed to lifestyle. For each biomarker, the adjusted model was implemented as followsyi=0+lil+aia+sis+mim+ei(3)
Where m represents the effect of the potential mediator on the outcome variable (all other variables are as defined in model 1). For body fat percentage, which displayed lifestyle effects in females but not males (fig. S1), we modeled females only and removed the sex term (s) from models 1 and 3. For the composite measure of health, we used generalized linear models with a binomial link function instead of linear models.
To assess the significance of each mediating variable, we estimated the decrease in l in model 1 relative to model 3 across 1000 iterations of bootstrap resampling. We deemed a variable to be a significant mediator if the lower bound of the 95% confidence interval (for the decrease in l) did not overlap with 0. As a measure of effect size, we report the proportion of 1000 bootstrap resampling iterations for which the effect of lifestyle (l) was reduced when the potential mediating variable was included in the model (table S3); a proportion of >0.975 is equivalent to a 95% confidence interval that does not overlap with 0. As another measure of effect size, we report the percent change in the lifestyle effect estimated from model 1 relative to model 3 for each biomarker-mediator pair (without bootstrapping and using the full dataset to estimate each effect size; Fig. 3). Further, to understand the degree to which the total set of mediators that we identified explain the relationship between lifestyle and a given biomarker, we report the percent change in effect size for the lifestyle effect estimated from model 1 versus a model that included all the same covariates and all significant mediators for a given biomarker.
Testing for early-life effects on biomarkers of adult health. Several research groups (26, 27, 64, 65, 84) have operationalized the PAR and DC hypotheses by asking whether there is evidence for interaction effects between early-life and adult environments (in support of PAR) or whether early-life adversity is instead consistently associated with compromised adult health (in support of DC). We took a similar approach to disentangle these hypotheses. For biomarkers with no sex and lifestyle interaction, we first asked whether there was any evidence for interaction effects between adult lifestyle and lifestyle/urbanicity during early life using the following linear modelyi=0+lil+aia+sis+did+(lidi)ld+ei(4)where di represents the log10 population density for the birth location of individual i during their birth year, li represents adult lifestyle (urban versus rural), and ld captures the interaction effect between these two variables. For the two variables with lifestyle effects on health in females but not males (body fat percentage and blood glucose levels), we modeled females only and removed the sex term (s). For the composite measure of health, we used generalized linear models with a binomial link function instead of linear models. For each of the 11 models, we extracted the P value associated with ld and corrected for multiple hypothesis testing (79). In all cases, the nominal and FDR-corrected P value was >0.05, suggesting that PARs do not explain early-life effects on health in the Turkana.
Next, we reran the appropriate version of model 4 for each measure after removing the interaction effect (ld). For biomarkers with no sex and lifestyle interaction, this model was equivalent toyi=0+lil+aia+sis+did+ei(5)
For each of the 11 models, we extracted the P value associated with the early-life effect (d) and corrected for multiple hypothesis testing (79). We considered a given variable to show support for the DC hypothesis if the FDR-corrected P value was less than 0.05. Results for all models described in this section are presented in table S4A. Results for parallel analyses that use log10 population density for the sampling location to define the adult environment (rather than a binary urban/rural lifestyle variable) are presented in table S4B. All statistical analyses were performed in R (78).
W. Leonard, in Evolution in Health and Disease, S. Stearns, J. Koella, Eds. (Oxford Univ. Press, 2010), pp. 265276.
P. Baker, J. Hanna, T. Baker, The Changing Somoans: Behavior and Health in Transition (Oxford Univ. Press, 1986).
A. Grafen, in Reproductive success, T. H. Clutton-Brock, Ed. (University of Chicago Press, 1988), pp. 454471.
National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey Data from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) and the National Center for Health Statistics (NCHS) (2019) (Hyattsville, MD).
E. Fratkin, E. A. Roth, As Pastoralists Settle: Social, Health, and Economic Consequences of Pastoral Sedentarization in Marsabit District, Kenya (Kluwer Academic Publishers, 2004).
A.E. Caldwell, S. Eaton, M. Konner, in Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Medicine, M. Brune, W. Schiefenhoevel, Eds. (Oxford Univ. Press, 2019), pp. 209267.
D. E. Lieberman, The Story of the Human Body: Evolution, Health, and Disease (Pantheon Books, 2013).
G. Conroy, H. Pontzer, Reconstructing Human Origins: A Modern Synthesis (Norton, 2012).
T. McDade, C. Nyberg, in Human Evolutionary Biology, M. P. Muehlenbein, Ed. (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2010), pp. 581602.
Kenya STEPwise Survey for non communicable diseases risk factors (2015).
R Core Development Team, R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2015).
K. Galvin, M. Little, in Turkana Herders of the Dry Savanna: Ecology and Biobehavioral Response of Nomads to an uncertain Environment, M. A. Little, P. Leslie, Eds. (Oxford Univ. Press, 1999), pp. 125146.
M. A. Little, B. Johnson, in South Turkana Nomadism: Coping with an Unpredictably Varying Environment, R. Dyson-Hudson, J. T. McCabe, Eds. (HRAFlex Books, 1985).
R. J. Hijmans, raster: Geographic Data Analysis and Modeling. (2019).
R. J. Hijmans, geosphere: Spherical Trigonometry. (2017).
See the original post:
Urbanization and market integration have strong, nonlinear effects on cardiometabolic health in the Turkana - Science Advances
- Lose Weight Healthily And Quickly The Best Diet Plan And Daily Exercise [Last Updated On: June 10th, 2011] [Originally Added On: June 10th, 2011]
- How Can You Be Sure That A Diet Plan Will Really Work For You [Last Updated On: June 11th, 2011] [Originally Added On: June 11th, 2011]
- Diets That Work - The Best Diet Meals Online! [Last Updated On: June 11th, 2011] [Originally Added On: June 11th, 2011]
- Benefits Of A Healthy Lifestyle And How To Lose Weight With Healthy Diet Plans [Last Updated On: June 12th, 2011] [Originally Added On: June 12th, 2011]
- Healthy Foods That Any Succesful Diet Plan Should Include [Last Updated On: June 14th, 2011] [Originally Added On: June 14th, 2011]
- Want to Lose Weight Go for a Safe Diet Plan That Will Lead to Healthy Weight Loss [Last Updated On: June 14th, 2011] [Originally Added On: June 14th, 2011]
- 5 Components Of A Successful Exercise And Diet Plan [Last Updated On: June 14th, 2011] [Originally Added On: June 14th, 2011]
- Healthy Eating Tips to Lose Weight [Last Updated On: June 14th, 2011] [Originally Added On: June 14th, 2011]
- Fad Diets: Do They Really Work? [Last Updated On: June 14th, 2011] [Originally Added On: June 14th, 2011]
- How To Avoid Yo-Yo Diet Plans [Last Updated On: June 17th, 2011] [Originally Added On: June 17th, 2011]
- Diets that work fast [Last Updated On: June 17th, 2011] [Originally Added On: June 17th, 2011]
- Set Up the Perfect Diet Plan with These Simple Tips [Last Updated On: June 17th, 2011] [Originally Added On: June 17th, 2011]
- diets that work [Last Updated On: June 19th, 2011] [Originally Added On: June 19th, 2011]
- Finally, Weight-loss Diets that Work? Medicine Matters from Sandra Fryhofer, MD [Last Updated On: June 19th, 2011] [Originally Added On: June 19th, 2011]
- weight loss plans that work [Last Updated On: June 22nd, 2011] [Originally Added On: June 22nd, 2011]
- crash diets that work channel presents crash diets that work [Last Updated On: June 23rd, 2011] [Originally Added On: June 23rd, 2011]
- Diets that work fast - Protien Helps Burn Fats [Last Updated On: June 26th, 2011] [Originally Added On: June 26th, 2011]
- Diets That Work with Ali Fedotowsky [Last Updated On: June 27th, 2011] [Originally Added On: June 27th, 2011]
- Successful Weight Loss Secrets Revealed.avi [Last Updated On: June 29th, 2011] [Originally Added On: June 29th, 2011]
- Best Diet Plans Losing Weight by Changing Your Habits [Last Updated On: July 1st, 2011] [Originally Added On: July 1st, 2011]
- Finding Easy Diets That Work Fast [Last Updated On: July 4th, 2011] [Originally Added On: July 4th, 2011]
- Creating a Diet Plan That Can Give You Healthy Quick Weight Loss [Last Updated On: July 4th, 2011] [Originally Added On: July 4th, 2011]
- Can I lose Weight with a Free Diet Plan [Last Updated On: July 12th, 2011] [Originally Added On: July 12th, 2011]
- How to Find a Good Diet Plan [Last Updated On: July 13th, 2011] [Originally Added On: July 13th, 2011]
- Basic Dieting Tips Equal Successful Diet Plan [Last Updated On: July 18th, 2011] [Originally Added On: July 18th, 2011]
- Great Weight Loss Programs Basic Factors To Check [Last Updated On: July 21st, 2011] [Originally Added On: July 21st, 2011]
- Healthy Eating Tips You Should Add to Your Diet [Last Updated On: July 23rd, 2011] [Originally Added On: July 23rd, 2011]
- How Not to Cheat on your Diet Plan [Last Updated On: July 29th, 2011] [Originally Added On: July 29th, 2011]
- Choosing the Right Diet Plan for Permanent Weight Loss [Last Updated On: August 1st, 2011] [Originally Added On: August 1st, 2011]
- Healthy Dieting Tips How You Eat Is More Important Than What You Eat [Last Updated On: August 9th, 2011] [Originally Added On: August 9th, 2011]
- What Is Your Reason to Lose Weight [Last Updated On: August 13th, 2011] [Originally Added On: August 13th, 2011]
- The Basics Of A Safe Diet Plan For Your Heart [Last Updated On: August 17th, 2011] [Originally Added On: August 17th, 2011]
- Eating the Right Foods will Give You a Diet Plan that Works [Last Updated On: August 25th, 2011] [Originally Added On: August 25th, 2011]
- Diets That Don't Work [Last Updated On: September 8th, 2011] [Originally Added On: September 8th, 2011]
- Liquid Protein Diets Work [Last Updated On: September 9th, 2011] [Originally Added On: September 9th, 2011]
- Do Low Carb Diets Work ? [Last Updated On: September 9th, 2011] [Originally Added On: September 9th, 2011]
- Why Diets Don't Work [Last Updated On: September 12th, 2011] [Originally Added On: September 12th, 2011]
- A DIET that WORKS: NO DIET! A RAW FOOD LIFESTYLE ~ for OSCAR! ~ BIKE TOURING the WEST COAST! vid#46 [Last Updated On: September 12th, 2011] [Originally Added On: September 12th, 2011]
- Quick Diets Really Work [Last Updated On: September 12th, 2011] [Originally Added On: September 12th, 2011]
- Diets Dont Work - Thinking Thin Does [Last Updated On: September 13th, 2011] [Originally Added On: September 13th, 2011]
- Low Carb Diet-Why Low Carb Diets Work [Last Updated On: September 14th, 2011] [Originally Added On: September 14th, 2011]
- Atkins Diet: Does low carb work as well for all groups? [Last Updated On: September 16th, 2011] [Originally Added On: September 16th, 2011]
- How Does The HCG Diet Work - Official HCG diet Plan - HCG Diet For Men - HCG Diet User Reviews [Last Updated On: September 19th, 2011] [Originally Added On: September 19th, 2011]
- Crash diets that work from worlds best diet channel [Last Updated On: September 24th, 2011] [Originally Added On: September 24th, 2011]
- Macro-Patterning: Advanced Carb Cycling For Fat Loss [Last Updated On: September 24th, 2011] [Originally Added On: September 24th, 2011]
- Why Low Calorie Diets Don't Work and Never Will (Don't Starve or Crash Diet) [Last Updated On: September 25th, 2011] [Originally Added On: September 25th, 2011]
- Why "fad" diets don't work? [Last Updated On: September 27th, 2011] [Originally Added On: September 27th, 2011]
- The Crazy Celebrity Cabbage Soup Diet That Works [Last Updated On: September 29th, 2011] [Originally Added On: September 29th, 2011]
- Weight loss diet that (really) works [Last Updated On: September 29th, 2011] [Originally Added On: September 29th, 2011]
- Fad Diets Do Not Work. How To Lose Weight Without Dieting... [Last Updated On: October 1st, 2011] [Originally Added On: October 1st, 2011]
- Low Calorie Diet DO NOT WORK - Lose 30lbs Manipulating Them [Last Updated On: October 2nd, 2011] [Originally Added On: October 2nd, 2011]
- Fad Diets: What works and What DOESN'T? [Last Updated On: October 4th, 2011] [Originally Added On: October 4th, 2011]
- All Diets Work [Last Updated On: October 4th, 2011] [Originally Added On: October 4th, 2011]
- 3 Weight Loss Diet Tips That Works | Fastest Way To Lose Weight [Last Updated On: October 4th, 2011] [Originally Added On: October 4th, 2011]
- Do Diet Pills Work? INSPIRED: The Movie Expert Insights [Last Updated On: October 4th, 2011] [Originally Added On: October 4th, 2011]
- Fad Diets: What Really Works for Weight Loss [Last Updated On: October 5th, 2011] [Originally Added On: October 5th, 2011]
- Healthy Diets Work Fast - Free Diet Video [Last Updated On: October 5th, 2011] [Originally Added On: October 5th, 2011]
- Does Hcg Diet Plan Work - A Peek Into Hcg Diet Plan [Last Updated On: October 5th, 2011] [Originally Added On: October 5th, 2011]
- Researchers Discover Why High-Fat, Low-Carb Ketogenic Diets Work To Control Epileptic Seizures [Last Updated On: October 5th, 2011] [Originally Added On: October 5th, 2011]
- Why "Quick Fix" Diets Do Not Work! [Last Updated On: October 5th, 2011] [Originally Added On: October 5th, 2011]
- Binge Eating [Last Updated On: October 5th, 2011] [Originally Added On: October 5th, 2011]
- Rosie Huntington-Whiteley Diet Weight Measurements [Last Updated On: October 5th, 2011] [Originally Added On: October 5th, 2011]
- does the diet solution program really work [Last Updated On: October 5th, 2011] [Originally Added On: October 5th, 2011]
- EASIEST Diet For Women to Follow (Don't Starve Yourself Thin!) [Last Updated On: October 6th, 2011] [Originally Added On: October 6th, 2011]
- Crash diets that really work [Last Updated On: October 6th, 2011] [Originally Added On: October 6th, 2011]
- Cabbage Soup Diet [Last Updated On: October 6th, 2011] [Originally Added On: October 6th, 2011]
- Do Detox Diets Really Work? [Last Updated On: October 6th, 2011] [Originally Added On: October 6th, 2011]
- Does the every other day diet really work? [Last Updated On: October 6th, 2011] [Originally Added On: October 6th, 2011]
- Why Extreme Diets Like Atkins Low Carb and HCG Diet DONT WORK LONG TERM [Last Updated On: October 6th, 2011] [Originally Added On: October 6th, 2011]
- Fast Weight Loss Diet: Does Calorie Counting Work? [Last Updated On: October 6th, 2011] [Originally Added On: October 6th, 2011]
- Beyond Chocolate. Diets don't work! [Last Updated On: October 8th, 2011] [Originally Added On: October 8th, 2011]
- Failure To Thrive: GFCF and Feingold Diets Work For ... [Last Updated On: October 10th, 2011] [Originally Added On: October 10th, 2011]
- Low Carb Diets: How They Work [Last Updated On: October 10th, 2011] [Originally Added On: October 10th, 2011]
- Diet Pills That Work - Video [Last Updated On: October 14th, 2011] [Originally Added On: October 14th, 2011]
- Diets That Work - Lose 30lbs In 30 Days - Video [Last Updated On: October 15th, 2011] [Originally Added On: October 15th, 2011]
- Understanding the Atkins Diet, Why It Works and What it Can Do For You - Video [Last Updated On: October 15th, 2011] [Originally Added On: October 15th, 2011]
- crash diets that work channel presents crash diets that work - Video [Last Updated On: October 15th, 2011] [Originally Added On: October 15th, 2011]
- REALISTIC Diet [Last Updated On: October 15th, 2011] [Originally Added On: October 15th, 2011]
- Why Diets Don't Work - Video [Last Updated On: October 15th, 2011] [Originally Added On: October 15th, 2011]
- Diets That Work - 'Burn' Fat Faster With This Diet - Video [Last Updated On: October 15th, 2011] [Originally Added On: October 15th, 2011]